Isto para mim foi sempre óbvio, mas como costumo dizer o óbvio não é igual para todas as pessoas. Sempre considerei que a mobilização dos pais para a educação era a variável desprezada da actuação política na política da educação , e que a necessidade dessa falta ser remediada seria, de modo inequívoco, ainda mais imperiosa em regiões com as características dos Açores: escrevi isso em 1996 (e coloquei neste blogue: usar o motor de busca para ver as notas relativas a "educação" - são três notas de 17.01.2010 sob a epígrafe "Algo escrito sobre a educação para a Convenção...").
Num debate público, alguns anos atrás, afirmei que a eficácia das políticas públicas dependia em grande parte da adesão dos cidadãos (dos pais no caso da educação) à sua concretização. Fui apelidado de anti-democrático. Enfim...
Ao fim juntei ainda outro artigo que qualifica este no bom sentido (atenção ao bold com o sublinhado) e lembra que, em todo o caso, a introdução do pré-primário em Portual foi uma excelente aposta na potencialização dos resultados do nosso sistema educativo.
Ao fim juntei ainda outro artigo que qualifica este no bom sentido (atenção ao bold com o sublinhado) e lembra que, em todo o caso, a introdução do pré-primário em Portual foi uma excelente aposta na potencialização dos resultados do nosso sistema educativo.
How About Better Parents? - NYTimes.com
IN recent years, we’ve been treated to reams of op-ed articles about how we need better teachers in our public schools and, if only the teachers’ unions would go away, our kids would score like Singapore’s on the big international tests. There’s no question that a great teacher can make a huge difference in a student’s achievement, and we need to recruit, train and reward more such teachers. But here’s what some new studies are also showing: We need better parents. Parents more focused on their children’s education can also make a huge difference in a student’s achievement.
European Children More Likely to Outperform Parents Than Americans - Real Time Economics - WSJ
¶ How do we know? Every three years, the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, or O.E.C.D., conducts exams as part of the Program for International Student Assessment, or PISA, which tests 15-year-olds in the world’s leading industrialized nations on their reading comprehension and ability to use what they’ve learned in math and science to solve real problems — the most important skills for succeeding in college and life. America’s 15-year-olds have not been distinguishing themselves in the PISA exams compared with students in Singapore, Finland and Shanghai.
¶ To better understand why some students thrive taking the PISA tests and others do not, Andreas Schleicher, who oversees the exams for the O.E.C.D., was encouraged by the O.E.C.D. countries to look beyond the classrooms. So starting with four countries in 2006, and then adding 14 more in 2009, the PISA team went to the parents of 5,000 students and interviewed them “about how they raised their kids and then compared that with the test results” for each of those years, Schleicher explained to me. Two weeks ago, the PISA team published the three main findings of its study:
¶ “Fifteen-year-old students whose parents often read books with them during their first year of primary school show markedly higher scores in PISA 2009 than students whose parents read with them infrequently or not at all. The performance advantage among students whose parents read to them in their early school years is evident regardless of the family’s socioeconomic background. Parents’ engagement with their 15-year-olds is strongly associated with better performance in PISA.”
¶ Schleicher explained to me that “just asking your child how was their school day and showing genuine interest in the learning that they are doing can have the same impact as hours of private tutoring. It is something every parent can do, no matter what their education level or social background.”
¶ For instance, the PISA study revealed that “students whose parents reported that they had read a book with their child ‘every day or almost every day’ or ‘once or twice a week’ during the first year of primary school have markedly higher scores in PISA 2009 than students whose parents reported that they had read a book with their child ‘never or almost never’ or only ‘once or twice a month.’ On average, the score difference is 25 points, the equivalent of well over half a school year.”
¶ Yes, students from more well-to-do households are more likely to have more involved parents. “However,” the PISA team found, “even when comparing students of similar socioeconomic backgrounds, those students whose parents regularly read books to them when they were in the first year of primary school score 14 points higher, on average, than students whose parents did not.”
¶ The kind of parental involvement matters, as well. “For example,” the PISA study noted, “on average, the score point difference in reading that is associated with parental involvement is largest when parents read a book with their child, when they talk about things they have done during the day, and when they tell stories to their children.” The score point difference is smallest when parental involvement takes the form of simply playing with their children.
¶ These PISA findings were echoed in a recent study by the National School Boards Association’s Center for Public Education, and written up by the center’s director, Patte Barth, in the latest issue of The American School Board Journal.
¶ The study, called “Back to School: How parent involvement affects student achievement,” found something “somewhat surprising,” wrote Barth: “Parent involvement can take many forms, but only a few of them relate to higher student performance. Of those that work, parental actions that support children’s learning at home are most likely to have an impact on academic achievement at school.
¶ “Monitoring homework; making sure children get to school; rewarding their efforts and talking up the idea of going to college. These parent actions are linked to better attendance, grades, test scores, and preparation for college,” Barth wrote. “The study found that getting parents involved with their children’s learning at home is a more powerful driver of achievement than parents attending P.T.A. and school board meetings, volunteering in classrooms, participating in fund-raising, and showing up at back-to-school nights.”
¶ To be sure, there is no substitute for a good teacher. There is nothing more valuable than great classroom instruction. But let’s stop putting the whole burden on teachers. We also need better parents. Better parents can make every teacher more effective.
The study compares the U.S. and various countries including Australia, Canada and the major European countries, and attempts to measure to what degree a child’s economic future is tied to their parents’ education, income, IQ and various other measures. Not surprisingly, parental education is closely tied to their children’s economic success everywhere. But the tie was strongest in the U.S., meaning children born to more-educated parents are much more likely to move up the ladder.What’s notable is that the divergent paths between children of the educated and not starts from the very beginning — “as early as they can first be measured,” according to the Pew report. A child’s economic future is closely tied to both their parents’ wits and socio-economic standing, but, underscoring the entrenched role social norms play in education, the link was higher for the former. There was no country where children from high and low education parents started out equally prepared for school, a noble but impossible goal.Echoing economic research that has shown large payoffs for government investment in early education, the Pew study shows that the eventual wage and education gap between children of educated and non-educated parents was narrower in Denmark and France, both countries with universal preschool.
Sem comentários:
Enviar um comentário